Working memory is a higher cognitive construct with limited capacity responsible for maintaining and processing relevant information required in everyday activities proceeded in a goal-directed fashion (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974; Baddeley, 2012; Baddeley et al., 2021). For instance, working memory is involved when someone is asked to recall the last two instruction points for the second stage of an activity such as a game. In this scenario, information is retrieved from long-term memory and processed in working memory to perform the task.

Relationship with Other Cognitive Constructs

Results from earlier studies demonstrate strong and consistent associations between working memory measures and other higher-order cognitive constructs. These include reasoning (Kane & Engle, 2002), fluid intelligence (Chuderski, 2013; Oberauer et al., 2005), and attention (Unsworth et al., 2014). These constructs play a crucial role in learning outcomes and everyday life abilities such as reading comprehension, math, and problem-solving (Shah & Miyake, 1999; Diamond & Ling, 2020).

Working Memory and Executive Functions

It is well established from various studies that working memory capacity measures strongly correlate (r = .97) with executive functions (EFs) (McCabe et al., 2010). Executive functions are a set of high-level control processes required by individuals to successfully thrive in the real world. The family of executive functions includes abilities such as updating, inhibitory control, and cognitive flexibility (set shifting) (Stievano & Valeri, 2013; Diamond, 2015; Friedman & Miyake, 2018).

Impact on Self-Regulation and Goal-Directed Behavior

Executive functions are responsible for an individual’s self-regulation in goal-directed behavior due to their influence on low-level processes (Friedman & Miyake, 2018). They impact each aspect of human existence, from social behavior to academic achievements (Diamond, 2015). However, EFs are fragile and prone to external environmental influences. Impairment to any of the EF abilities can significantly impact an individual’s quality of life.

Working Memory and Intelligence

Research has shown that working memory and fluid intelligence share a substantial amount of their latent variance, ranging from 50% to 85% (Kane et al., 2005; Oberauer et al., 2005). This strong relationship implies that working memory is closely associated with critical thinking, which involves the ability to solve complex problems through mental operations such as identifying relationships and drawing inferences (Dwyer et al., 2014; Ennis, 2018).

Role in Critical Thinking

Empirical research has indicated that working memory plays an important role in various aspects of critical thinking ability, such as deductive reasoning and the inhibition of thinking biases. For example, Noone et al. (2016) found that working memory, measured by tone monitoring and letter-memory tasks, predicted critical thinking ability, including argument analysis, verbal reasoning, and hypothesis testing skills.

Developmental Perspective

From a developmental standpoint, post-error slowing (PES) in executive function tasks has been observed in children as young as 3-4 years of age (Jones et al., 2003). PES appears to be coarser in younger children and becomes fine-tuned with increasing age and experience (Brewer & Smith, 1989; Dubravac et al., 2021; Roebers, 2022). This developmental trajectory suggests that the relationship between working memory, executive functions, and higher-order cognitive processes continues to evolve throughout childhood and adolescence.

 

References

Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1-29. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422

Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 8, pp. 47-89). Academic Press.

Baddeley, A., Finch, J. P., & Allen, R. J. (2021). Working memory: The state of the science. Annual Review of Psychology, 72, 1-25. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010419-051352

Brewer, N., & Smith, G. A. (1989). Developmental changes in processing speed: Influence of speed-accuracy regulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118(3), 298-310. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.118.3.298

Chuderski, A. (2013). When are fluid intelligence and working memory isomorphic and when are they not? Intelligence, 41(4), 244-262. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.04.003

Diamond, A. (2015). Effects of physical exercise on executive functions: Going beyond simply moving to moving with thought. Annals of Sports Medicine and Research, 2(1), 1011.

Diamond, A., & Ling, D. S. (2020). Review of the evidence on, and fundamental questions about, efforts to improve executive functions, including working memory. In J. M. Novick, M. F. Bunting, M. R. Dougherty, & R. W. Engle (Eds.), Cognitive and working memory training: Perspectives from psychology, neuroscience, and human development (pp. 143-431). Oxford University Press.

Dubravac, M., Roebers, C. M., & Meier, B. (2021). Age differences in post-error slowing across tasks with varying cognitive demands. Developmental Psychology, 57(8), 1284-1298. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001213

Dwyer, C. P., Hogan, M. J., & Stewart, I. (2014). An integrated critical thinking framework for the 21st century. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 12, 43-52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2013.12.004

Ennis, R. H. (2018). Critical thinking across the curriculum: A vision. Topoi, 37(1), 165-184. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-016-9401-4

Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2018). Unity and diversity of executive functions: Individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex, 86, 186-204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2016.04.023

Jones, L. B., Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (2003). Development of executive attention in preschool children. Developmental Science, 6(5), 498-504. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00307

Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2002). The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual-differences perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(4), 637-671. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196323

Kane, M. J., Hambrick, D. Z., & Conway, A. R. A. (2005). Working memory capacity and fluid intelligence are strongly related constructs: Comment on Ackerman, Beier, and Boyle (2005). Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 66-71. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.66

McCabe, D. P., Roediger III, H. L., McDaniel, M. A., Balota, D. A., & Hambrick, D. Z. (2010). The relationship between working memory capacity and executive functioning: Evidence for a common executive attention construct. Neuropsychology, 24(2), 222-243. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017619

Noone, C., Bunting, B., & Hogan, M. J. (2016). Does mindfulness enhance critical thinking? Evidence for the mediating effects of executive functioning in the relationship between mindfulness and critical thinking. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 2043. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02043

Oberauer, K., Schulze, R., Wilhelm, O., & Süß, H.-M. (2005). Working memory and intelligence—Their correlation and their relation: Comment on

As a research scientist specialising in cognitive neuroscience and psychology, I write a blog that explores the fascinating world of computational modelling and gamified Working Memory training. Through my writing, I share insights from my research on how these interventions affect learning and cognitive functions in both typically developing individuals and clinical populations. My blog delves into cognitive rehabilitation for people with brain injuries, neurodegenerative disorders, and neurodevelopmental conditions. I also discuss my work on assessing cognition, emotion, and behaviour, as well as understanding the biopsychosocial factors that impact everyday cognitive abilities. By translating complex scientific concepts into accessible content, I aim to provide a valuable resource for professionals and the general public interested in brain health and cognitive science.

Dorota Styk
The Author