Schizophrenia is a complex psychiatric disorder characterised by a range of symptoms that significantly impact an individual’s thoughts, perceptions, emotions, and behaviour. According to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5), schizophrenia is defined by the presence of two or more of the following symptoms for at least one month (or less if successfully treated), with at least one of them being delusions, hallucinations, or disorganised speech (American Psychiatric Association, 2013):

  1. Delusions
  2. Hallucinations
  3. Disorganised speech
  4. Grossly disorganised or catatonic behaviour
  5. Negative symptoms (e.g., diminished emotional expression)

Additionally, the DSM-5 criteria require that these symptoms cause significant impairment in one or more major areas of functioning, such as work, interpersonal relations, or self-care, for a significant portion of time since the onset of the disturbance. The signs of the disorder must persist for at least six months, including at least one month of active-phase symptoms (Tandon et al., 2013).

It is important to note that the DSM-5 has made several changes to the diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia compared to previous editions. For instance, the special status previously given to bizarre delusions and Schneiderian first-rank auditory hallucinations has been removed. Furthermore, the DSM-5 has eliminated the classical subtypes of schizophrenia (paranoid, disorganised, catatonic, undifferentiated, and residual) due to their limited diagnostic stability, low reliability, and poor validity (Tandon et al., 2013).

The lifetime prevalence of schizophrenia is estimated to be between 0.3% and 0.7% (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). The onset of psychotic symptoms typically occurs between the mid-teens and mid-30s, with the peak age of onset for the first psychotic episode being in the early to mid-20s for males and late 20s for females (Häfner et al., 1998).

Cognitive Deficits in Schizophrenia

Cognitive impairments are widely recognised as a core feature of schizophrenia, with deficits observed across multiple domains. Two areas of particular interest in schizophrenia research are working memory and executive functions, both of which play crucial roles in daily functioning and are often impaired in individuals with the disorder (Millan et al., 2012).

Working Memory in Schizophrenia

Working memory refers to the cognitive system responsible for temporarily holding and manipulating information. It is crucial for learning, reasoning, and language comprehension. Numerous studies have consistently demonstrated that individuals with schizophrenia exhibit significant deficits in working memory compared to healthy controls (Lee & Park, 2005).

A comprehensive meta-analysis conducted by Lee and Park (2005) examined working memory performance across various tasks in individuals with schizophrenia. The results revealed large deficits in working memory across all three domains: phonological, visuospatial, and central executive. These findings suggest that working memory impairment is a robust and pervasive feature of schizophrenia.

The deficits in working memory observed in schizophrenia are not limited to a specific modality or task. Impairments have been documented in both verbal and visual-spatial working memory tasks. For instance, individuals with schizophrenia often struggle with tasks that require the maintenance and manipulation of auditory, visual, lexical, or semantic information (Barch & Ceaser, 2012).

Neuroimaging studies have provided insights into the neural basis of working memory deficits in schizophrenia. Evidence suggests that there are disturbed frontotemporal interactions during working memory tasks in patients with schizophrenia (Perlstein et al., 2001). These alterations in brain activation patterns may contribute to the observed impairments in working memory performance.

Executive Functions in Schizophrenia

Executive functions encompass a set of higher-order cognitive processes that enable goal-directed behaviour, including planning, inhibition, cognitive flexibility, and decision-making. Deficits in executive functions are considered a hallmark of cognitive impairment in schizophrenia and have significant implications for daily functioning and overall quality of life (Orellana & Slachevsky, 2013).

Research has consistently demonstrated that individuals with schizophrenia exhibit impairments across various domains of executive functioning. A critical review by Orellana and Slachevsky (2013) highlighted the following key findings regarding executive dysfunctions in schizophrenia:

  1. Planning and organisation: Patients with schizophrenia often struggle with tasks that require strategic planning and organisation of behaviour (Velligan et al., 2000).
  2. Attention and inhibition: Deficits in selective attention and inhibitory control are commonly observed, affecting the ability to focus on relevant information and suppress irrelevant stimuli (Nuechterlein et al., 2008).
  3. Cognitive flexibility: Individuals with schizophrenia frequently demonstrate difficulties in adapting to changing task demands and shifting between different cognitive sets (Miyake et al., 2000).
  4. Working memory: As discussed earlier, working memory impairments are a prominent feature of executive dysfunction in schizophrenia (Lee & Park, 2005).
  5. Problem-solving: Patients often exhibit difficulties in generating and implementing effective strategies for problem-solving tasks (Seidman et al., 2010).
  6. Decision-making: Impairments in decision-making processes, particularly in situations involving uncertainty or emotional components, have been reported in schizophrenia (Bechara et al., 1994; Bechara et al., 1999).

The severity of executive function deficits in schizophrenia has been found to correlate with both negative symptoms and disorganisation (Martino et al., 2007). This suggests a potential link between the clinical presentation of the disorder and the underlying cognitive impairments.

Recent research has also focused on distinguishing between “cool” and “hot” executive functions in schizophrenia. “Cool” executive functions refer to cognitive processes that are relatively emotion-neutral, such as working memory and planning. In contrast, “hot” executive functions involve emotional or motivational components, such as decision-making in emotionally charged situations or theory of mind. A study by García-Fernández et al. (2020) found that patients with schizophrenia performed worse on both cool and hot executive function tasks compared to healthy controls. This suggests that executive dysfunction in schizophrenia extends beyond purely cognitive domains and encompasses socio-emotional aspects as well.

Working Memory Training in Schizophrenia

Given the significant impact of working memory deficits on daily functioning in schizophrenia, there has been growing interest in developing interventions to improve working memory performance. Working memory training programmes have shown promise in various populations, including healthy individuals and those with other cognitive disorders. However, the efficacy of such interventions in schizophrenia remains a topic of ongoing research and debate (Wykes et al., 2011).

Several studies have investigated the effects of working memory training in individuals with schizophrenia, with mixed results. Some key findings from these studies include:

  1. Improved visual and verbal memory: A study by Hubacher et al. (2013) found that four weeks of working memory training led to improvements in both visual and verbal memory in patients with chronic schizophrenia. This suggests that targeted cognitive training may have the potential to enhance specific aspects of memory functioning in this population.
  2. Long-term effects: Subramaniam et al. (2014) reported increased working memory performance six months after a 16-week working memory training programme. This finding is particularly encouraging as it indicates that the benefits of cognitive training may persist over time.
  3. Brain activation changes: A meta-analysis by Li et al. (2015) revealed that working memory training was associated with changes in brain activation patterns in individuals with schizophrenia. This suggests that cognitive training may have the potential to induce neuroplastic changes in the brain regions involved in working memory processing.
  4. Limited transfer effects: While some studies have shown improvements in trained working memory tasks, the transfer of these gains to untrained tasks or other cognitive domains has been less consistent. For example, a randomised controlled trial by Jahshan et al. (2019) found that working memory training led to significant gains in untrained processing speed tasks but showed inconsistent effects on far-transfer tasks.
  5. Domain-specific improvements: The study by Jahshan et al. (2019) also highlighted that processing speed training led to significant gains in untrained processing speed tasks, suggesting that domain-specific cognitive training may be more effective than targeting working memory alone.
  6. Potential mediating factors: Some researchers have suggested that the efficacy of working memory training in schizophrenia may be mediated by factors such as intrinsic motivation (Medalia & Saperstein, 2013). This highlights the importance of considering individual differences and motivational factors when designing and implementing cognitive training interventions.

While these findings provide some evidence for the potential benefits of working memory training in schizophrenia, it is important to note that the research in this area is still limited and often characterised by methodological variations across studies. Furthermore, the mechanisms by which cognitive training may enhance cognition and functioning in schizophrenia are not yet fully understood (Vinogradov et al., 2012).

Implications and Future Directions

The extensive cognitive deficits observed in schizophrenia, particularly in working memory and executive functions, have significant implications for treatment and rehabilitation approaches. Understanding these impairments is crucial for developing targeted interventions that can improve cognitive functioning and, ultimately, enhance overall quality of life for individuals with schizophrenia (McGurk et al., 2007).

The promising results from working memory training studies suggest that cognitive remediation may be a valuable adjunctive treatment for schizophrenia. However, several important considerations and future directions emerge from the current state of research:

  1. Personalised interventions: Given the heterogeneity of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia, future research should focus on developing personalised cognitive training programmes that target specific areas of impairment for each individual (Wykes et al., 2011).
  2. Combination approaches: Investigating the potential synergistic effects of combining cognitive training with other therapeutic approaches, such as pharmacological treatments or psychosocial interventions, may lead to more comprehensive and effective treatment strategies (Vinogradov et al., 2012).
  3. Neural mechanisms: Further research is needed to elucidate the neural mechanisms underlying cognitive improvements following working memory training. This could involve longitudinal neuroimaging studies to track changes in brain structure and function over the course of cognitive interventions (Subramaniam et al., 2012).
  4. Ecological validity: Developing and evaluating cognitive training programmes that more closely resemble real-world tasks and situations may enhance the transfer of gains to daily functioning (Medalia & Saperstein, 2013).
  5. Long-term outcomes: More longitudinal studies are needed to assess the durability of cognitive improvements and their impact on functional outcomes over extended periods (Subramaniam et al., 2014).
  6. Motivational factors: Exploring the role of motivation in cognitive training outcomes and developing strategies to enhance engagement and adherence to training programmes may improve the efficacy of these interventions (Medalia & Saperstein, 2013).
  7. Integration with clinical care: Investigating ways to integrate cognitive training into routine clinical care for individuals with schizophrenia could help bridge the gap between research findings and practical applications (McGurk et al., 2007).

In conclusion, while significant progress has been made in understanding the nature and extent of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia, particularly in working memory and executive functions, much work remains to be done. The development and refinement of effective cognitive training interventions hold promise for improving the lives of individuals with schizophrenia. However, continued research is needed to optimise these approaches and translate them into meaningful clinical outcomes (Wykes et al., 2011).

References

 

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.

Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50(1-3), 7-15.

Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Damasio, A. R., & Lee, G. P. (1999). Different contributions of the human amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex to decision-making. Journal of Neuroscience, 19(13), 5473-5481.

Bechara, A., Dolan, S., & Hindes, A. (2002). Decision-making and addiction (part II): myopia for the future or hypersensitivity to reward? Neuropsychologia, 40(10), 1690-1705.

Beck, S. J., Hanson, C. A., Puffenberger, S. S., Benninger, K. L., & Benninger, W. B. (2010). A controlled trial of working memory training for children and adolescents with ADHD. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 39(6), 825-836.

Brüne, M. (2001). Social cognition and psychopathology in an evolutionary perspective. Psychopathology, 34(2), 85-94.

Brüne, M. (2003). Theory of mind and the role of IQ in chronic disorganized schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 60(1), 57-64.

Buschkuehl, M., Jaeggi, S. M., & Jonides, J. (2012). Neuronal effects following working memory training. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, S167-S179.

Catalan, A., Angosto, V., Díaz, A., Valverde, C., de Artaza, M. G., Sesma, E., … & Gonzalez-Torres, M. A. (2018). Relation between psychotic symptoms, theory of mind and mentalization in schizophrenia. Psicothema, 30(4), 408-414.

Cortese, S., Ferrin, M., Brandeis, D., Buitelaar, J., Daley, D., Dittmann, R. W., … & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. (2015). Cognitive training for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: meta-analysis of clinical and neuropsychological outcomes from randomized controlled trials. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 54(3), 164-174.

Fonseca-Pedrero, E., Paino, M., Lemos-Giráldez, S., & Muñiz, J. (2015). Schizotypal traits and depressive symptoms in nonclinical adolescents. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 56, 203-211.

García-Fernández, L., Cabot-Ivorra, N., Romero-Ferreiro, V., Pérez-Martín, J., & Rodriguez-Jimenez, R. (2020). “Cool” and “Hot” executive functions in patients with a predominance of negative symptoms of schizophrenia. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 571271.

Grace, J., & Malloy, P. F. (2001). Frontal Systems Behavior Scale (FrSBe): Professional manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Hubacher, M., Weiland, M., Calabrese, P., Stoppe, G., Stöcklin, M., Fischer-Barnicol, D., … & Penner, I. K. (2013). Working memory training in patients with chronic schizophrenia: a pilot study. Psychiatry Journal, 2013, 154867.

Jahshan, C., Vinogradov, S., Wynn, J. K., Hellemann, G., & Green, M. F. (2019). A randomized controlled trial of a working memory and processing speed training program for adults with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 208, 447-453.

Jang, S. K., Kim, S., Kim, C. Y., Lee, H. S., & Choi, K. H. (2017). Attentional processing of emotional faces in schizophrenia: Evidence from eye tracking. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 126(3), 286-300.

Keefe, R. S., Goldberg, T. E., Harvey, P. D., Gold, J. M., Poe, M. P., & Coughenour, L. (1992). The Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia: reliability, sensitivity, and comparison with a standard neurocognitive battery. Schizophrenia Research, 68(2-3), 283-297.

Klingberg, T., Forssberg, H., & Westerberg, H. (2002). Training of working memory in children with ADHD. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24(6), 781-791.

Lee, J., & Park, S. (2005). Working memory impairments in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(4), 599-611.

Li, X., Xiao, Y. H., Zhao, Q., Leung, A. W., Cheung, E. F., & Chan, R. C. (2015). The neuroplastic effect of working memory training in healthy volunteers and patients with schizophrenia: Implications for cognitive rehabilitation. Neuropsychologia, 75, 149-162.

Martino, D. J., Bucay, D., Butman, J. T., & Allegri, R. F. (2007). Neuropsychological frontal impairments and negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 152(2-3), 121-128.

McGurk, S. R., Twamley, E. W., Sitzer, D. I., McHugo, G. J., & Mueser, K. T. (2007). A meta-analysis of cognitive remediation in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164(12), 1791-1802.

Medalia, A., & Saperstein, A. M. (2013). Does cognitive remediation for schizophrenia improve functional outcomes? Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 26(2), 151-157.

Millan, M. J., Agid, Y., Brüne, M., Bullmore, E. T., Carter, C. S., Clayton, N. S., … & Young, L. J. (2012). Cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: characteristics, causes and the quest for improved therapy. Nature Reviews Drug Discovery, 11(2), 141-168.

Minzenberg, M. J., Laird, A. R., Thelen, S., Carter, C. S., & Glahn, D. C. (2009). Meta-analysis of 41 functional neuroimaging studies of executive function in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66(8), 811-822.

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49-100.

Nuechterlein, K. H., Green, M. F., Kern, R. S., Baade, L. E., Barch, D. M., Cohen, J. D., … & Marder, S. R. (2008). The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery, part 1: test selection, reliability, and validity. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(2), 203-213.

Orellana, G., & Slachevsky, A. (2013). Executive functioning in schizophrenia. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 4, 35.

Owen, A. M., McMillan, K. M., Laird, A. R., & Bullmore, E. (2005). N-back working memory paradigm: A meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 25(1), 46-59.

Perlstein, W. M., Carter, C. S., Noll, D. C., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Relation of prefrontal cortex dysfunction to working memory and symptoms in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(7), 1105-1113.

Ragland, J. D., Laird, A. R., Ranganath, C., Blumenfeld, R. S., Gonzales, S. M., & Glahn, D. C. (2009). Prefrontal activation deficits during episodic memory in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166(8), 863-874.

Reichenberg, A., & Harvey, P. D. (2007). Neuropsychological impairments in schizophrenia: Integration of performance-based and brain imaging findings. Psychological Bulletin, 133(5), 833-858.

Seidman, L. J., Giuliano, A. J., Meyer, E. C., Addington, J., Cadenhead, K. S., Cannon, T. D., … & Woods, S. W. (2010). Neuropsychology of the prodrome to psychosis in the NAPLS consortium: relationship to family history and conversion to psychosis. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(6), 578-588.

Snitz, B. E., MacDonald III, A. W., & Carter, C. S. (2006). Cognitive deficits in unaffected first-degree relatives of schizophrenia patients: a meta-analytic review of putative endophenotypes. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 32(1), 179-194.

Subramaniam, K., Luks, T. L., Fisher, M., Simpson, G. V., Nagarajan, S., & Vinogradov, S. (2012). Computerized cognitive training restores neural activity within the reality monitoring network in schizophrenia. Neuron, 73(4), 842-853.

Subramaniam, K., Luks, T. L., Garrett, C., Chung, C., Fisher, M., Nagarajan, S., & Vinogradov, S. (2014). Intensive cognitive training in schizophrenia enhances working memory and associated prefrontal cortical efficiency in a manner that drives long-term functional gains. Neuroimage, 99, 281-292.

Tandon, R., Gaebel, W., Barch, D. M., Bustillo, J., Gur, R. E., Heckers, S., … & Carpenter, W. (2013). Definition and description of schizophrenia in the DSM-5. Schizophrenia Research, 150(1), 3-10.

Tost, H., Alam, T., & Meyer-Lindenberg, A. (2010). Dopamine and psychosis: theory, pathomechanisms and intermediate phenotypes. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(5), 689-700.

Velligan, D. I., Bow-Thomas, C. C., Huntzinger, C., Ritch, J., Ledbetter, N., Prihoda, T. J., & Miller, A. L. (2000). Randomized controlled trial of the use of compensatory strategies to enhance adaptive functioning in outpatients with schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(8), 1317-1323.

Vinogradov, S., Fisher, M., & de Villers-Sidani, E. (2012). Cognitive training for impaired neural systems in neuropsychiatric illness. Neuropsychopharmacology, 37(1), 43-76.

Wykes, T., Huddy, V., Cellard, C., McGurk, S. R., & Czobor, P. (2011). A meta-analysis of cognitive remediation for schizophrenia: methodology and effect sizes. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(5), 472-485.

As a research scientist specialising in cognitive neuroscience and psychology, I write a blog that explores the fascinating world of computational modelling and gamified Working Memory training. Through my writing, I share insights from my research on how these interventions affect learning and cognitive functions in both typically developing individuals and clinical populations. My blog delves into cognitive rehabilitation for people with brain injuries, neurodegenerative disorders, and neurodevelopmental conditions. I also discuss my work on assessing cognition, emotion, and behaviour, as well as understanding the biopsychosocial factors that impact everyday cognitive abilities. By translating complex scientific concepts into accessible content, I aim to provide a valuable resource for professionals and the general public interested in brain health and cognitive science.

Dorota Styk
The Author