Emotional Intelligence
Emotional intelligence (EI) refers to the ability to perceive, express, understand, and manage emotions (Mayer et al., 2004). It has been conceptualised in various ways, primarily falling into three main models: ability-based, trait-based, and mixed models (Pérez-González et al., 2021). The ability model, developed by Mayer and Salovey, views EI as a form of innate intelligence comprising several capacities that influence how people understand and manage their own emotions and those of others. These emotion processing skills include: (1) perception, evaluation and expression of emotions, (2) emotional facilitation of thought, (3) understanding and analysis of emotions, and (4) reflective regulation of emotions (Mayer et al., 2016).
The trait model, proposed by Petrides and Furnham, conceptualises EI as a constellation of emotional self-perceptions located at the lower levels of personality hierarchies (Pérez-González et al., 2021). The mixed model, exemplified by Goleman’s work, combines both ability and trait aspects of EI (Goleman, 1998).
Research has demonstrated that EI plays a crucial role in various aspects of life, particularly in professional settings. Studies have shown that EI can help change employee attitudes and behaviours in jobs involving emotional demands by increasing job satisfaction and reducing job stress (Pérez-González et al., 2021). Moreover, EI has been found to be related to less psychological distress and can serve to develop resilience, which is a protective variable against psychological distress (Pérez-González et al., 2021).
Trauma
Trauma is defined as an event, series of events, or set of circumstances experienced by an individual as physically or emotionally harmful or threatening and that has lasting adverse effects on the individual’s functioning and mental, physical, social, emotional, or spiritual well-being (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2014). Traumatic events can occur at various levels, including individual (e.g., childhood and adult physical, sexual, and emotional abuse or neglect), family (e.g., parental loss, incarceration, mental illness), community (e.g., experiences of racism, xenophobia, and discrimination), and historical (e.g., slavery, genocide, colonisation).
The impact of trauma, particularly when experienced during critical periods of development, can be profound and long-lasting. The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACEs) study identified categories of adversities experienced by age 18 that can lead to significant health challenges later in life (Felitti et al., 1998). Research has shown that individuals with multiple ACEs are at significantly higher risk for various physical and mental health issues, including cardiovascular disease, chronic respiratory disease, depression, and substance abuse (Hughes et al., 2017).
Working Memory Performance
Working memory refers to the ability to simultaneously manipulate and store information (Cowan, 2017). It plays a crucial role in processes involving language comprehension, reasoning, and planning, making it a key component in analysing cognition. The concept of working memory evolved from the classical understanding of short-term memory, as proposed by Baddeley and Hitch in 1974 (Baddeley, 2000).
Working memory performance can be measured through various tasks, such as complex span tasks, n-back tasks, and running memory span tasks (Conway et al., 2005). These tasks assess an individual’s ability to manipulate and store information simultaneously, with higher scores indicating better working memory performance.
The Interplay Between Emotional Intelligence, Trauma, and Working Memory Performance
Research has revealed significant connections between emotional intelligence, trauma, and working memory performance. These relationships have important implications for understanding human cognition and behaviour.
Impact of Trauma on Emotional Intelligence
Gottfredson and Becker (2021) argue that past psychological trauma impacts later emotional intelligence capabilities. They present evidence that psychological trauma impairs the brain areas and functions that support EI. Specifically, they found that psychological trauma can impair the Salience Network (SN) and the Default Mode Network (DMN), which are crucial for emotional processing and regulation.
The SN, including areas such as the amygdala and anterior insula, is the primary hub for the feeling, awareness, and processing of emotions, as it is in charge of automatic emotional attention (Gottfredson & Becker, 2021). People with physical damage to the brain areas within the SN have been found to have a diminished ability to detect fearful and other negative facial expressions, recognize dramatic music intended to evoke fear, experience the feelings of fear, be reliable and reputable partners, extend the appropriate level of trust to others, and demonstrate empathy and control aggression (Gottfredson & Becker, 2021).
The DMN plays a strong role in the interpretation, recognition, and regulation of emotion (Gottfredson & Becker, 2021). People who have experienced physical damage to the brain areas within the DMN have diminished ability to emotionally regulate oneself, engage in appropriate social behaviour, be self-aware, engage in self-sight or self-reflection, and be focused (as opposed to reactive) (Gottfredson & Becker, 2021).
These findings suggest that trauma can significantly impair an individual’s emotional intelligence capabilities, potentially leading to lower EI. This connection provides a potential explanation for why EI might vary across individuals and suggests a method for improving EI through healing from past trauma.
Impact of Mood on Working Memory Performance
Studies have shown that mood can significantly impact working memory performance. Rączy and Orzechowski (2021) discovered that participants induced into a positive mood performed better on a working memory task compared to those induced into a negative mood. Specifically, they found that happiness (positive mood) led to better performance than sadness (negative mood).
Interestingly, the emotional valence of words used in working memory tasks also impacts performance. Participants performed better when processing positive or negative words compared to emotionally neutral words. This suggests that emotional content, whether positive or negative, may enhance working memory processing (Rączy & Orzechowski, 2021).
The Role of Anger in Working Memory Performance
While much research has focused on the effects of happiness and sadness on working memory, less is known about the impact of anger. Some evidence suggests that anger may function similarly to happiness in enhancing working memory capacity. Bodenhausen et al. (1994, as cited in Rączy & Orzechowski, 2021) found that both happy and angry moods increased stereotypical thinking, enhanced reliance on pre-existing knowledge structures, and showed a higher tendency to judge social information that conformed with pre-existing beliefs and expectations.
These findings imply that happiness and anger may have similar effects on executive control, which includes working memory. However, more research is needed to fully understand the relationship between anger and working memory performance.
Long-term Effects of Trauma on Cognitive Performance
A study by Lachman and Lynch (2020) found that individuals who suffer trauma in childhood and adulthood may experience a greater amount of cognitive decline as they age than individuals who haven’t experienced trauma. The research, published in the Journal of Traumatic Stress, also found that recent trauma suffered in adulthood has a larger impact on some aspects of cognitive functioning than trauma in childhood.
The study involved roughly 2,500 adults, ages 28 to 84, who were tested on their cognitive abilities in two areas: executive functioning (EF) and episodic memory (EM). Those respondents who said they had experienced more traumatic events showed greater declines in both EF and EM over the course of nine years (Lachman & Lynch, 2020).
Chiasson et al. (2021) conducted an exploratory fMRI study examining the influence of emotion on working memory among men with histories of childhood sexual abuse (CSA). The study provided preliminary empirical evidence of the impact CSA can have on men regarding working memory when negative stimuli are involved. This research highlights the long-lasting effects of childhood trauma on cognitive functioning, particularly in the context of emotional processing.
Conclusion
The interplay between emotional intelligence, trauma, and working memory performance is complex and multifaceted. Trauma can significantly impact an individual’s emotional intelligence, potentially leading to lower EI capabilities. This, in turn, can affect various aspects of cognitive functioning, including working memory performance.
Understanding these relationships is crucial for developing effective interventions and support strategies. For instance, addressing past trauma may be a key component in improving emotional intelligence. Similarly, recognising the impact of mood on working memory performance can inform strategies for optimising cognitive function in various settings, from education to the workplace.
Future research should continue to explore these connections, particularly focusing on the long-term effects of trauma on emotional intelligence and cognitive performance. Additionally, investigating potential interventions that can mitigate the negative impacts of trauma on EI and working memory could provide valuable insights for improving mental health and cognitive functioning across diverse populations.
References
Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 417-423.
Chiasson, C., Moorman, J., Romano, E., Vezarov, M., Cameron, C., & Tessel, R. (2021). The influence of emotion on working memory: An fMRI investigation of sexual abuse in men. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 34(1), 207-218.
Conway, A. R., Kane, M. J., Bunting, M. F., Hambrick, D. Z., Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user’s guide. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(5), 769-786.
Cowan, N. (2017). The many faces of working memory and short-term storage. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(4), 1158-1170.
Felitti, V. J., Anda, R. F., Nordenberg, D., Williamson, D. F., Spitz, A. M., Edwards, V., & Marks, J. S. (1998). Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults: The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 14(4), 245-258.
Goleman, D. (1998). Working with emotional intelligence. Bantam.
Gottfredson, R. K., & Becker, W. J. (2021). Past trauma and future workplace behavior: A review and examination of the psychological effects of trauma and its implications for organizations. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 42(1), 57-78.
Hughes, K., Bellis, M. A., Hardcastle, K. A., Sethi, D., Butchart, A., Mikton, C., Jones, L., & Dunne, M. P. (2017). The effect of multiple adverse childhood experiences on health: a systematic review and meta-analysis. The Lancet Public Health, 2(8), e356-e366.
Lachman, M. E., & Lynch, C. D. (2020). Trauma and cognitive functioning: Findings from a population-based study of adults. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 33(5), 643-653.
Mayer, J. D., Caruso, D. R., & Salovey, P. (2016). The ability model of emotional intelligence: Principles and updates. Emotion Review, 8(4), 290-300.
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2004). Emotional intelligence: Theory, findings, and implications. Psychological Inquiry, 15(3), 197-215.
Pérez-González, J. C., Saklofske, D. H., & Mavroveli, S. (2021). Emotional intelligence: Foundations, assessment, and human performance. Springer.
Rączy, K., & Orzechowski, J. (2021). The impact of mood on verbal working memory capacity. Memory, 29(5), 625-633.
Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. (2014). SAMHSA’s concept of trauma and guidance for a trauma-informed approach. HHS Publication No. (SMA) 14-4884. Rockville, MD: Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration.
As a research scientist specialising in cognitive neuroscience and psychology, I write a blog that explores the fascinating world of computational modelling and gamified Working Memory training. Through my writing, I share insights from my research on how these interventions affect learning and cognitive functions in both typically developing individuals and clinical populations. My blog delves into cognitive rehabilitation for people with brain injuries, neurodegenerative disorders, and neurodevelopmental conditions. I also discuss my work on assessing cognition, emotion, and behaviour, as well as understanding the biopsychosocial factors that impact everyday cognitive abilities. By translating complex scientific concepts into accessible content, I aim to provide a valuable resource for professionals and the general public interested in brain health and cognitive science.
Dorota Styk
The Author